top of page


Parents have a duty to discipline their children. When children do something wrong, their parents should discipline them to teach a lesson. 


One of the most common manifestations of discipline is physical punishment, otherwise known as corporal punishment. Corporal punishment can refer to a variety of actions, from slapping, spanking, and even prolonged periods in a fixed position. However, this form of punishment has raised questions regarding effectiveness and ethics. How effective is corporal punishment? What are its effects? Is it morally just to administer such punishment on a child? 


In schools across the United States, corporal punishment is still considered a valid form of punishment in 19 states. Corporal punishment is more prevalent in states in the South, such as Alabama, Arkansas, and Mississippi (National Institute of Health). In a second study published by the National Institute of Health, researchers surveyed a group of parents bringing in their children to a clinic, asking about their position on corporal punishment. Based on this survey, 51% of these parents showed support for forms of corporal punishment, 37% disagreed, and the rest took no clear position. Dovetailing this finding, Columbia University’s Demographic and Health Surveys show that in 2015, nearly 40% of children under the age of five were exposed to some form of physical punishment in their lives. Surveys from the United Nations International Children's Emergency Fund (UNICEF) show that hundreds of millions of 2-to-4 year olds still experience corporal punishment globally. Corporal punishment remains a common choice for parents as a form of discipline in homes, with almost one third of U.S parents reporting spanking their children weekly. 


Clearly, corporal punishment is common—but is it also effective? Psychologist Elizabeth Thompson Gershoff of the National Center for Children in Poverty at Columbia University conducted a meta-analysis in which over the course of 62 years, she attempted to unveil the consequences of corporal punishment. In her study, Gershoff discovered that for most cases, corporal punishment was linked to continuous negative behavior in kids and did not seem to have an overwhelmingly positive result. However, it was also highlighted in her study that there did seem to be an immediate, temporary obedience from the children after they received the punishment, though this did not last. Gershoff finds that the severity and how often a child is punished plays a significant factor in determining its outcome, where the more often and harder the punishment, the more likely an aggressive behavior begins to show. This underscores how subjective corporal punishment can be, with many different variables being at play to determine the outcome of punishment inflicted on children. Ultimately, however, Gershoff’s studies have found that there is a correlation between corporal punishment and negative behaviors among children and advises parents to avoid administering such discipline.


Children need to understand boundaries and I think that children need to understand that there should be punishments… in direct proportion to the improper behavior that they might demonstrate.

Conversely, many parents still argue that corporal punishment is necessary and an effective way to discipline children. For instance, an article published by Britannica argued that corporal punishment is a valid form of punishment when used in moderation. The article explained how more moderate punishments, such as a time-out, would not be effective on more severe behavior. A sharper form of punishment, such as corporal punishment, would overall yield better results in teaching children a lesson when considering certain forms of behavior. Moreover, the same article argued that the physical consequence better establishes boundaries and forces children to continue to behave in the future. President and Dean of the Charles H. Mason Theological Seminary Harold Bennet mentions that “children need to understand boundaries and I think that children need to understand that there should be punishments… in direct proportion to the improper behavior that they might demonstrate.” Overall, the debate surrounding corporal punishment has still remained complex and polarizing. It is crucial for parents, educators, and policymakers to weigh both sides of the debate and strive to create an environment for children to learn, grow, and develop. 


Writer's pictureBrandon Choi


You wake up in the morning and start to get ready, going through your daily morning. Brushing your teeth, taking a shower, eating breakfast, getting dressed. You then get ready to leave the house, putting shoes on and deciding on how to make your commute. All of these rudimentary actions involve critical cognitive skills. From deciding on what to eat and what to wear, to remembering what commute is the fastest, to simple things such as knowing to brush your teeth involve cognitive skills. Such skills such as thinking, judging, and remembering play a major role in human life. Cognitive processes, also known as cognitive functions or skills, are the psychological functions the brain performs in order to take in and process information received. Information is received through the five basic senses.  This information is then analyzed, stored, and then used in making relevant decisions in everyday life. 



Cognitive processes, also known as cognitive functions or skills, are the psychological functions the brain performs in order to take in and process information received.


BAISC COGNITION

 


There are two types of cognitive processes: basic and higher. Basic cognition refers to capacities, such as attention, perception,  information, and memory. Attention is the action of selecting and focusing on stimuli in order to retain the information. The five senses are focused and used to interact with the environment around a person. One may use their eyesight and hearing  to learn during a psychology class, or smell and taste to pick up distinctive characteristics of a food. Perception is the processing of the information given by the five senses and through attention. The brain captures sensations and gives them meaning, associating something picked up to an item, idea, or a concept. This information is then compiled together  to create an organized space to access this information. This leads to the final type of basic cognitive process, memory. Stored information is retained within the system for later access. This varies from both short-term and long-term memory, depending on how the brain sorts the information. 



HIGHER-LEVEL CONGNITION

 

The second type of cognitive process is higher-level cognition. This involves actions, such as critical thinking, creativity, language, and learning. Thinking is the bringing together of information, leading to meaningful judgments and deductions. People can plan for their futures, make important decisions, and manage their behavior by using the previously stored information in new ways. Creativity is the development of new ideas based upon what is already in one’s system. When using previously accessible information, sometimes people begin to create their own path and use that information to develop more advanced  ideas or concepts. Language is the fundamental method for communication, and knowing how to speak a language is important for living life. In general, humans communicate with one another using speech, reading, and writing, though there are also gesture-based languages, such as sign language. Finally, learning is the process in which humans do everything in life. The taking in of information and using it in everyday life is something that we cannot live without and is an important cognitive process. 



However, there exists a number of biases that affect how people utilize cognitive processes, due to systematic errors that occur when shortcuts, or quick rules, are used when using cognitive processes.


When executing these actions, however, there exists a number of biases that affect how people utilize cognitive processes, due to systematic errors that occur when shortcuts, or quick rules, are used when using cognitive processes. These are known as cognitive biases. The most common five biases are anchoring bias, confirmation bias, negativity bias, actor-observer bias, and the halo effect.


Anchoring bias involves relying excessively on the initial information received when making decisions, even if it’s irrelevant or incorrect. People tend to go with their natural instinct and what is first presented to them. For instance, in shopping. If you see a shirt first retailing for $1,200 then see another shirt for $500, it is not uncommon for the second shirt to be viewed as cheap because in comparison to the first $1,200, it seems drastically cheaper. By first taking in the information of a shirt being $1,200 and internalizing that, and then seeing a $500 shirt, the expectations for the normal price of a shirt are shot up because you are anchored on the first price.


The second type of bias, confirmation bias, is the inclination to favor information that supports existing beliefs and values while disregarding contradictory information. Once again, humans can be considered very self-centered and assume that they are always correct. As a result, when looking into information on a topic with pre-existing knowledge, people tend to favor information and sources that agree with what they initially believed and fail to look at opposing arguments closely, if any at all. For example, when looking into new smartphones, there may be a pre-existing, strong bias for a certain brand of smartphone. When reading reviews, recommendations, and opinions, it is common to subconsciously pay more attention to positive information about that brand or product. This, in turn, clouds the true facts and leads to a stronger inclination to prefer an object or idea due to this pre-existing bias.


Negativity bias is the tendency to give more importance to negative information or experiences compared to positive ones. Negative events, emotions, or information is usually paid more attention to and often have a stronger impact on thoughts and behaviors. Memories, for instance, tend to be more vivid and clear with more negative experiences rather than positive ones. Humiliation, embarrassment, and anger are all recalled more vividly because of its negative qualities and stand out more as a result. News stations also use this to their advantage, opting to display more negative stories as it captures attention and keeps the audience engaged. Stories involving conflict, natural disasters, and bad news are focused on more as it keeps the audience watching and intrigued. 


The fourth type of bias, actor-observer bias, is the cognitive bias that refers to the tendency of people to attribute their own behavior to external factors that are out of their control. On the contrary, people often attribute others’ behaviors to their personality and other internal factors. For instance, in traffic, if you cut someone off, you might tell yourself that you were just in a hurry or you didn’t see the car. However, when someone else cuts you off, it is not uncommon for the reaction to be to blame the other person, to call them careless, reckless, and an aggressive driver. 


The horn and halo effect make up the fifth type of cognitive bias. The halo effect is where our overall positive impression of a person influences how we feel and think about other parts of their character. If we perceive someone positively in one aspect, we are more likely to have a positive impression of them as a whole, allowing the positive trait to "radiate" and create a favorable "halo" around the person. Conversely, the horn effect is the opposite, where an initial negative impression results in continuous negative views about that person. This bias can lead to judgments that are not entirely objective, as our perception is influenced by a single trait. The most common instance is with physical attractiveness. If we find a person especially appealing, we often presume other positive qualities, such as kindness and intelligence, but this may not always be the case. This bias is also the basis for the idea of “having a good first impression,” where the initial interaction with a person can either set up or ruin the way that the other person visualizes the rest of your character and you as a person.

Updated: Dec 27, 2023

By Brandon Choi

How and why are we so much more intelligent, and how do humans learn?

In all the grandeur of Mother Nature, we humans stand out as remarkable beings, distinguished by a unique characteristic that sets us apart from the rest of the animal kingdom: our unparalleled intelligence. This intellectual superiority renders humans as the superior species on Earth, allowing us to essentially rule over everything. But where does this intelligence begin? Why are we so much more intelligent, and how do humans learn? This leads into the study of cognitive development, defined by Stanford Childrens’ Medicine as “the growth of a child’s ability to think and reason.” Understanding cognitive development may allow for more informed decisions on the most optimal way that people learn. Cognitive psychologist Jean Piaget and Lev Vygtosky attempts to answer these questions and provide their insights on how humans retain information.



Imagine a video game—a virtual realm of human knowledge. Each level is something to conquer, something to learn. One starts at the beginning with the tutorial and slowly works their way up, completing level by level and slowly increasing in difficulty. This allows the player to complete the video game in small, manageable stages that are acclimated to their current skill set. They slowly progress through the levels until they reach the final level and ultimately beat the game. This is how Piaget viewed the process in which humans learned—in stages. Piaget claimed that knowledge is slowly built upon completing the stages, much like experience points and in-game currency one may gather for completing levels and tasks. Piaget broke these stages down into four main categories: the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete operational, and the formal operational stages. He wrote that these four stages are different levels of sophistication to which children have access and should be taught based on a child’s given stage. 



SENSORIMOTOR STAGE

 

The first stage, the sensorimotor stage, begins at birth and lasts until around two years old, but of course, this varies from child to child. In this stage, Piaget found that children tend to be extremely curious, whether it be to explore that new room, smell that new candle, or try that new food, children in the sensorimotor stage engage in very basic actions to learn. By engaging in these actions, they base their learning heavily on their five senses. As a result, Piaget noted that infants develop a variety of abilities, such as deferred imitation, representational play, and self-recognition. They start to become aware of the world around them and not just their own body. They also begin to do small things intentionally. Later in this stage, Piaget noted that infants will begin to understand object permanence, the idea that even if they cannot see an object, it doesn’t necessarily mean that it doesn’t exist. This then leads to the development of the memory, and the storing of information, recalling that information, and labeling it. 



PREOPERATIONAL STAGE

 

As the child nears two years old, they enter the preoperational stage, typically lasting five years from ages two to seven. In this stage, thinking is still heavily influenced by the way things appear rather than using logical reasoning. Piaget found that children in this stage do not understand conservation yet, the idea that even if the shape of something changes to seem smaller that the amount remains constant. As the child begins to speak and communicate, they remain curious and seek to gain answers not only through their senses, but also through communication and questioning. Children were noted to be very egocentric in this stage, unable to understand that everyone has different views on the world outside of what they can comprehend. During the preoperational stage, children begin to form vast imaginations and use symbolism to comprehend the world easier. This often leads to the creation of “imaginary friends” and animism, where children give life to inanimate objects to make them seem alive. Piaget also found that as the child progresses through this stage, their egocentric nature slowly declines and they begin to enjoy being around others, often interacting with others and eventually making friends.



CONCRETE OPERATIONAL STAGE

 

At around age 7, lasting up until age 11, the child hits the third stage, known as the concrete operational stage. Here, logic becomes an important factor in the child’s development. As a result, children gain the ability to inductively deduce and reason. This is representative of Piaget’s theory, as they first began as simply observing around them and now have “leveled-up” to where they are able to connect what they see and use it to make conclusions. As their logical thinking improves, children have been noted to be able to begin to solve problems and work things out in their head instead of using the physical world to aid them. Mental reversal of objects and concepts become possible, also connected with stronger image processing. For instance, children are generally able to imagine a box being broken down and folded back up to its original shape, or vice versa. The concept of conservation has become understandable, that no matter the shape as long as the original amount of something is still there it is the same size. Moreover, Piaget observed that children become less egocentric and begin to consider others' feelings. They become empathetic and sympathetic toward not only other people, but toward other living creatures as well, beginning to understand their own and others’ emotions. 


FORMAL OPERATIONAL STAGE

 

Finally, children reach the formal operational stage at around 12 years old. This stage, according to Piaget, lasts from age 12 into adulthood for the rest of their life. Here, children master abstract ideas and are able to accurately describe different ideas clearly. It becomes easier for them to take in new abstract ideas as well. Children understand complex concepts without needing a specific example to connect statements and visualize what they need to understand. Piaget noted that they are able to tackle hypothetical problems and overall think on a deeper level than they were able to before. The formal operational stage is where the child has beaten the game and has acquired all of the skills, all of the tools, and all of the upgrades necessary to tackle any problem thrown at them in the future.


cognitive development in children is heavily influenced by a child’s culture, social factors, and ultimately their upbringing.



Lev Vygotsky, however, provides insight on another method in which children may develop and learn. His theories can be visualized through an image of a building under construction. In the construction of this building, each story is built with meticulously planned materials and assistance. The help and work of construction workers and machines alike allow for the building to be fully built and reach its full potential, its maximum height. As learning develops, the level of assistance is adjusted in order to more efficiently erect the building. Vygotsky’s theories were based on a similar system; a system of constant adjusting and accommodating for the growth of cognitive development. Vygotsky proposed that cognitive development in children is heavily influenced by a child’s culture, social factors, and ultimately their upbringing.



CULTURAL FACTORS

 

Conversely, unlike Piaget, Vygotsky proposed that cognitive development in children is heavily influenced by a child’s culture, social factors, and ultimately their upbringing. Vygotsky, however, first mentioned that infants are all born with basic abilities for intellectual development and growth, which he called “elementary mental functions.” These include attention, sensation, perception, and memory. Through social interaction, these functions develop and become what he calls higher mental functions. These social interactions would vary between cultures and ultimately determine a child’s order of thinking. Because cultures can be vastly different, Vygotsky noted that therefore different cultures produce children who grow up with a variety of different skills in different cultures. Vygotsky defines the term tools of intellectual adaptation, which refer to means of thinking that children learn through socialization within their respective cultures. Children growing up in 2 different societies and cultures may have completely different views on a subject simply because of how they were raised and how they were taught to view the world around them. For instance, Western cultures like the United States’ promotes a value of individuality among people and that standing out is a good thing. For instance, individualism is notably prominent in our advertisement system. The main selling point of a product will often be to stand out, to be better than those around you, to be different. Conversely, many East Asian countries, such as Japan, promote ideas of conformity. Many of their advertisements urge the purchase of a product because everyone else is buying it, more often than not including a celebrity promoting the product.



SOCIAL INFLUENCES

 

During the process of socialization among cultures, Vygotsky identified a key figure: the presence of a more knowledgeable other,” as he calleds it, also known as an MKO. According to Vygotsky, an MKO can be anyone in the child’s environment who has more knowledge than the child. This includes but is not limited to people such as a parent, teacher, sibling, peer, or even a stranger. As explained by Vygotsky, a successful MKO should be able to guide the child to learn what is possible within their proximal zone, or the area between what a child can do independently and what can achieve with guidance. Vygotsky argues that without social interaction, children will still develop—just not to their full potential. An MKO should ultimately help the child explore their proximal zone and optimize their learning experience. This is explained by an example of scaffolding. Here, it is described that the actions of an MKO’s actions should align with how scaffolding works, where the MKO provides the students with the tools they need in order to obtain information and learn, to achieve higher levels of thinking, similar to how scaffolding helps a building reach higher heights. 



LINGUISTIC CONTRIBUTION

 

Vygotsky also discussed the importance of language among the learning processes. He believed that language develops from socialization and interactions, for the purpose of communication. He believed that language was humankind’s greatest tool for communicating to the outside world. According to Vygotsky, language holds two important roles in cognitive development. He stated that language was the main means of communication where adults, or an MKO, could transmit information to children. The second purpose of language was a tool for intellectual adaptation, where language would make gaining information easier the more it was absorbed and learnt. Within language, Vygotsky categorizes it into 3 different forms: social speech, private speech, and private speech goes “underground.”


Social speech is the simple means of communication that we all should be familiar with. It is defined by Vygotsky as the form of external communication that is mainly used to talk to others and used to socialize. This typically begins from around the age of two.

The second form, private speech, is the form of speech which is directed to the self. This form of speech mainly serves as an intellectual function, as well as serving the purpose of self-regulation. Private speech has been typically observed to emerge at around the age of three.

Finally, private speech goes “underground.” This is defined as when private speech becomes inaudible and takes on the form of silent inner speech. This form occurs inside of the brain and is only heard by the person speaking, and is usually noticed in children around the age of seven. Vygotsky also discussed a process called reciprocal teaching. In reciprocal teaching, both the child and the MKO are engaging in conversation through the use of the different language forms and take turns leading the conversation. This results in the building of ideas and knowledge. Vygotsky found that this results in a heightened ability to learn from one’s social surroundings and language. He highlighted the importance of language in a child’s learning process , and suggested that language is one of the most important aspects of a child’s upbringing that contributes to their learning.


While Piaget emphasizes self-initiated discovery and individual progression through distinct stages, Vygotsky stresses the importance of social interactions, cultural influences, and the guidance of more knowledgeable others in shaping cognitive abilities.

Ultimately, Piaget and Vygotsky each presented distinct perspectives on human cognitive development. While Piaget emphasized self-initiated discovery and individual progression through distinct stages, Vygotsky stressed the importance of social interactions, cultural influences, and the guidance of more knowledgeable others in shaping cognitive abilities. Both theories contributed valuable insights to our understanding of how humans learn.





bottom of page